Artigos Científicos

Exposure to violence during childhood is associated with telomere erosion from 5 to 10 years of age: a longitudinal study

I Shalev; et al

5 de maio de 2013

Molecular Psychiatry (2013) 18, 576–581

Exposure to violence during childhood is associated with telomere erosion from 5 to 10 years of age: a longitudinal study

I Shalev1,2, T E Moffitt1,2,3,4, K Sugden1,2,3,4, B Williams1,2,3,4, R M Houts1,2, A Danese4,5, J Mill4, L Arseneault4 and A Caspi1,2,3,4

  1. 1Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA
  2. 2Institute for Genome Sciences and Policy, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA
  3. 3Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA
  4. 4Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, London, UK
  5. 5Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, London, UK

Correspondence: Dr I Shalev, Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Suite 201 Grey House, 2020 West Main Street, Box 104410, Durham, NC 27708, USA. E-mail:idan.shalev@duke.edu

Top

Abstract

There is increasing interest in discovering mechanisms that mediate the effects of childhood stress on late-life disease morbidity and mortality. Previous studies have suggested one potential mechanism linking stress to cellular aging, disease and mortality in humans: telomere erosion. We examined telomere erosion in relation to children's exposure to violence, a salient early-life stressor, which has known long-term consequences for well-being and is a major public-health and social-welfare problem. In the first prospective-longitudinal study with repeated telomere measurements in children while they experienced stress, we tested the hypothesis that childhood violence exposure would accelerate telomere erosion from age 5 to age 10 years. Violence was assessed as exposure to maternal domestic violence, frequent bullying victimization and physical maltreatment by an adult. Participants were 236 children (49% females; 42% with one or more violence exposures) recruited from the Environmental-Risk Longitudinal Twin Study, a nationally representative 1994–1995 birth cohort. Each child's mean relative telomere length was measured simultaneously in baseline and follow-up DNA samples, using the quantitative PCR method forT/S ratio (the ratio of telomere repeat copy numbers to single-copy gene numbers). Compared with their counterparts, the children who experienced two or more kinds of violence exposure showed significantly more telomere erosion between age-5 baseline and age-10 follow-up measurements, even after adjusting for sex, socioeconomic status and body mass index (B=−0.052, s.e.=0.021, P=0.015). This finding provides support for a mechanism linking cumulative childhood stress to telomere maintenance, observed already at a young age, with potential impact for life-long health.

Keywords: 

childhood stress; cumulative violence exposure; erosion; longitudinal; telomere length

Top

Introduction

Childhood stress is associated with later-life physical and mental-health problems.1, 2, 3, 4, 5 Interest in the etiological pathways that mediate the effect of early-life stress on physical and mental health has focused on key biological systems, including the sympathetic–nervous system, hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal axis, immune system and the epigenome, leading to important insights about the systemic effects of stress.6, 7, 8 But the questions of how and when childhood stress gets ‘under the skin’ at the cellular level, specifically in humans, remain to be answered.

In the past decade, the length of telomeres, the repetitive TTAGGG sequence at the end of linear chromosomes, has emerged as a promising new biomarker of stress.9 In the 1960s, it was discovered that fibroblast cells have a built-in mechanism that limits their replicative capacity, later named the ‘Hayflick limit’.10In the 1970s, it was suggested that telomeres are lost with each replication until a certain (Hayflick) limit, at which point the cell arrests and enters a state of senescence.11 Subsequent research confirmed that telomeres indeed get shorter with each division and established the utility of telomeres as a molecular clock for cellular replicative aging.12, 13

The advent of high-throughput laboratory techniques for measuring telomere length (TL) has opened the gate to new studies linking TL with a broad range of risk factors that predict disease morbidity, including smoking,14 obesity,14, 15, 16psychiatric disorders17, 18 and psychosocial stress.19, 20 It has thus been suggested that TL is a marker for biological aging (‘wear and tear’), rather than a clock for chronological aging.

Six recent studies have examined the possible association between childhood stress and TL. Four of these reported that adult cases who retrospectively recalled childhood adversity had shorter TL, as compared with adult controls.20,21, 22, 23 One study failed to replicate this association.24 In the only study of children, institutional care was associated with shorter TL in middle childhood.25

Although these studies advance understanding of the link between childhood stress and TL, their reliance on retrospective assessments of stress and cross-sectional designs raises important questions. Interpretation of findings from cross-sectional studies of TL is ambiguous in light of recent longitudinal analyses of repeated TL measurements. Longitudinal studies show that TL is highly variable across different age groups; that there is an inverse correlation between baseline TL and subsequent telomere erosion; and that, in some individuals, telomeres can lengthen over time.26, 27, 28, 29, 30, 31, 32 These recent findings indicate that the temporal process of telomere erosion is more complex than initially assumed, and that repeated measures (not just length at one time point) are needed to measure true telomere erosion in individuals who are experiencing stress.

Here, we used a longitudinal design to test the effects of violence exposure during childhood on telomere erosion in a cohort of young children. We assessed childhood adversity prospectively and measured TL at two time-points, at age-5 years and again at age-10 years. We assessed domestic violence, frequent bullying victimization and physical maltreatment. Based on evidence that the effects of stress are cumulative,33 we hypothesized that cumulative exposure to violence will be associated with accelerated TL erosion, already at a young age.

Top

Participants and methods

Participants

Participants were members of the Environmental-Risk (E-Risk) Study, which tracks the development of a birth cohort of 2232 British children. The sample was drawn from a larger birth register of twins born in England and Wales in 1994–1995.34 Full details about the sample are reported elsewhere.35 Briefly, the E-Risk sample was constructed in 1999–2000, when 1116 families with same-sex 5-year-old twins (93% of those eligible) participated in home-visit assessments. Families were recruited to represent the UK population of families with newborns in the 1990s, based on (a) residential location throughout England and Wales and (b) mother's age (that is, older mothers having twins via assisted reproduction were under-selected and teenage mothers with twins were over-selected). We used this sampling (a) to replace high-risk families who were selectively lost to the register via nonresponse and (b) to ensure sufficient numbers of children growing up in high-risk environments. Home visits were conducted when the children were aged 5, 7, 10 and, most recently, 12 years (96% participation). DNA samples were collected via buccal swabs at ages 5 and 10 years. The Joint South London and Maudsley and the Institute of Psychiatry NHS Ethics Committee (UK) approved each phase of the study.

As costs of measuring TL in all cohort children nationwide exceeded study resources, a subsample of 118 Caucasian families (236 children) was used for this study, comprising families who: (a) lived proximal to London (the city of our research lab), (b) had MZ twins, and (c) had violence exposure in one or both twins or (d) were lacking any violence exposure but matched on socioeconomic status and child sex.36, 37 The subsample is compared with the original cohort onTable 1.


Measurement of mean relative TL

DNA was extracted using standard procedures.38, 39, 40 Age-5 and age-10 DNA were stored at −80°C until assayed, to prevent degradation of the samples. All DNA samples were assayed for TL at the same time, independently of caseness and all operations were carried out by a laboratory technician blind to violence exposure.

TL was measured using a validated quantitative-PCR method,41 which determines mean TL across all chromosomes for all cells sampled (Supplementary Information). Briefly, the method involves two quantitative PCR reactions for each subject; one for a single-copy gene (S) and the other in the telomeric repeat region (T). Given each of these reactions is compared with a standard reference sample before the (T)/(S) ratio is calculated, the ratio will equal 1 when the experimental sample is identical to the reference sample. All DNA samples were run in triplicate for telomere and single-copy reactions at both age-5 and age-10, that is, 12 reactions per study subject.

Relative quantities of samples for each of the reactions were calculated as follows: first, reaction efficiencies (E) were calculated from the standard curve slope using E=10(1/−slope), where at 100% efficiency E=2. Next, relative quantities (RQs) were calculated using RQ=EΔCt where ΔCt is the difference between the average triplicate Ct value of the within-plate mean Ct, and the Ct value of the individual sample. Sample RQ values were calculated for each reaction separately (T and S). Finally, the TL relative to the amount of single-copy transcript was calculated using the ratio RQ(T)/RQ(S). The coefficient of variation (CV) for the triplicate samples was 0.97% for the telomere (T) reaction and 0.49% for the single-copy gene (S) reaction, indicating low measurement error.

Exposure to violence

We assessed three kinds of violence experiences between ages 5 and 10 years: exposure to domestic violence between the mother and her partner, frequent bullying victimization and physical maltreatment by an adult. We interviewed mothers (or the primary caregiver) about each exposure when the children were 5, 7 and 10 years of age and compiled a cumulative record of each child's exposure to violence. We have previously reported evidence on reliability and validity in this research for the measures of domestic violence,42 bullying victimization43, 44 and physical maltreatment by an adult.45, 46, 47

Domestic violence was assessed with the Conflict Tactics Scale,48 inquiring about 12 acts of physical violence (for example, kicking a partner, threatening a partner with a knife). The physical violence acts reported at three ages were summed and children who grew up in families who scored in the top 10% of the distribution of partner violence were coded as exposed to domestic violence. Of the children studied here, 17.0% (N=40) were exposed to domestic violence.

Bullying victimization was assessed by explaining to mothers that ‘someone is being bullied when another child (1) says mean and hurtful things, makes fun, or calls a person mean and hurtful names; (2) completely ignores or excludes someone from their group of friends or leaves them out of things on purpose; (3) hits, kicks, or shoves a person, or locks them in a room; (4) tells lies or spreads rumors about them; or (5) does other hurtful things like these. We call it bullying when these things happen often and it is difficult for the person being bullied to stop it happening. We do not call it bullying when it is done in a friendly or playful way.’ Narratives recorded by interviewers were later checked by an independent rater to verify that the events described were instances of bullying, operationally defined as evidence of repeated harmful actions between children where there was a power differential between the bully and the victim.43, 44 Of the children studied here, 24.2% (N=57) were frequently bullied.

Physical maltreatment by an adult was assessed using a standardized clinical interview protocol49 designed to enhance mothers’ comfort with reporting valid child maltreatment information, while also meeting researchers’ responsibilities for referral under the UK Children Act. No family has left the study following intervention. When mothers reported any maltreatment, interviewers followed with standardized probes (for example, accidental harm was ruled out, and sexual abuse was queried directly and was included in the measure). Over the years of data collection, the study maintained a cumulative dossier for each child, comprising recorded debriefings with interviewers who had coded any indication of maltreatment at any of the three successive home visits, recorded narratives of the three successive caregiver interviews and information from clinicians whenever the study made a referral. Based on review of each child's cumulative dossier, two clinical psychologists (TEM and the project coordinator) reached consensus for whether physical maltreatment had occurred. Examples of maltreatment in E-risk children included: the mother smacked the child weekly leaving marks or bruises; child was repeatedly beaten by a young adult step-sibling; child was routinely smacked by father when drunk ‘just to humiliate him’; child was fondled sexually and often slapped by the mothers’ boyfriend. Many, but not all cases identified in the course of our research were under investigation by police or social services, already on the child-protection register, or in foster care at follow-up, having been removed from their parents because of abuse. Of the children studied here, 26.7% (N=63) were maltreated by an adult.

Finally, we summed each type of violence exposure to create an index ofcumulative violence exposure: 54.2% (N=128) of the children were not violence exposed, 29.2% (N=69) were exposed to one type of violence (domestic violence,N=14; bullying victimization, N=28; physical maltreatment, N=27), and 16.5%(N=39) were exposed to two or more types of violence (domestic violence, N=26; bullying victimization, N=29; physical maltreatment, N=36).

Table 1 summarizes information about the characteristics of the sample and about groups of children who were victimized.

Covariates

Socioeconomic deprivation was constructed from a standardized composite of household income, parents’ highest education and parents’ highest occupational grade. Deprivation was defined as the lowest tertile of the distribution in the entire cohort.

Body mass index was constructed from parents’ reports of children's height and weight, and computed as weight (kg)/height (m2).

Children's health: Mothers reported about their children's health, on a four-point scale (0=poor; 3=very good); 8.9% (N=21) of the children were rated as 0 or 1. In addition, we collected information from mothers about each child's asthma, using an event history calendar for recording children's history of chronic health conditions; 12.3% (N=29) of the children were asthmatic.

Statistics

We tested the main hypothesis that violence exposure would be associated with telomere erosion between ages 5 and 10 years using ordinary least squares multiple regression analysis. The outcome variable was age-10 TL, controlling for baseline TL at age-5 years and sex, socioeconomic deprivation and body mass index at age-10 years as covariates. As each study family contains two children, all statistical analyses were corrected conservatively for the non-independence of the two twin observations per family, by using tests based on the sandwich or Huber/White variance estimator50 in SAS version 9.2 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA).

Top

Results

Descriptive statistics about TL during childhood

TL at age-5 years was significantly and positively correlated with TL at age-10 years (r=0.57, P<0.001; Figure 1). Despite this significant rank-order stability, there was also significant change. There was a significant decline in mean TL from age-5 to age-10 for the whole sample (General Linear Model repeated measures: F(353,1)=18.49, P<0.001). The average baseline TL at age-5 was 1.08 T/S(s.d.=0.44), and ranged from 0.02 to 3.07 T/S. Follow-up TL at age-10 averaged 0.96 T/S (s.d.= 0.28) and ranged from 0.06 to 1.71 T/S. These means are similar to those reported by the two other studies of TL in children.25, 51

Figure 1.
Figure 1 - Unfortunately we are unable to provide accessible alternative text for this. If you require assistance to access this image, please contact help@nature.com or the author

Association between telomere length at 5 and 10 years of age.

Full figure and legend (16K)Download PowerPoint slide (176 KB)

Does exposure to violence accelerate telomere erosion?

We tested our main hypothesis that cumulative violence exposure was associated with TL change during childhood. Children who experienced two or more types of violence exposure showed significantly accelerated TL erosion from baseline to follow-up measurement compared with children who had one type of violence exposure or who were not exposed to violence (B=−0.052, s.e.=0.021, P=0.015) (Figure 2). This association remained significant when we added controls for poor health and asthma to the analysis (B=−0.047, s.e.=0.021, P=0.028).

Figure 2.
Figure 2 - Unfortunately we are unable to provide accessible alternative text for this. If you require assistance to access this image, please contact help@nature.com or the author

Association between cumulative violence exposure and telomere length at 5 and 10 years of age.

Full figure and legend (13K)Download PowerPoint slide (180 KB)

We also tested the association between TL erosion and each violence exposure separately (Table 1). Consistent with the analysis of cumulative stress, the individual violence exposures all predicted more rapid telomere erosion, but this acceleration was statistically significant only in the case of exposure to physical maltreatment (for domestic violence, B=−0.056, s.e.=0.043, P=0.196; for bullying victimization, B=−0.037, s.e.=0.037 P=0.311; for physical maltreatment, B=−0.084, s.e.=0.035, P=0.018). In a supplementary analysis, we compared non-exposed children (N=128), children exposed only to physical maltreatment (N=27), and children with two or more exposures (including physical maltreatment and another violence exposure, N=36). The results showed that cumulative violence exposure was associated with TL change in childhood (B=−0.056, s.e.=0.024, P=0.020), implicating cumulative exposure rather than physical maltreatment alone.

Telomere lengthening?

Consistent with previous repeated-measures longitudinal studies,26, 27, 28, 29, 30,31 we observed telomere lengthening in a minority of children (Table 1). In addition, consistent with previous repeated-measures studies,26, 28, 31 shorter TL at baseline predicted more lengthening from age-5 to age-10 years (Figure 3,r=−0.77, P<0.001). Given uncertainty about the interpretation of telomere lengthening,29, 50 and the possibility that different mechanisms are likely to govern telomere shortening versus lengthening, we retested our main hypothesis of association between cumulative violence exposure and TL in individuals whose telomeres did not lengthen during the follow-up period. Following previous research,29 we defined lengthened telomeres as >15% increase in TL between measurements (16.9% of the sample). After excluding these children from the analysis, cumulative violence exposure remained significantly associated with accelerated telomere erosion at the same effect size (B=−0.057, s.e.=0.024,P=0.019).

Figure 3.
Figure 3 - Unfortunately we are unable to provide accessible alternative text for this. If you require assistance to access this image, please contact help@nature.com or the author

Association between baseline telomere length (TL) at age 5 years and TL change from 5 to 10 years of age.

Full figure and legend (15K)Download PowerPoint slide (169 KB)

Top

Discussion

This study shows for the first time that cumulative violence exposure is associated with accelerated TL erosion in children, from baseline to follow-up, for children who experienced violence at a young age. Adjusting for sex, socioeconomic deprivation and body mass index, as well as children's health, children who were exposed to multiple forms of violence had the fastest TL erosion rate.

The methodological strengths of this study include a longitudinal design with reliable and valid prospective assessments of multiple violence exposures during childhood and repeated measurements of TL during this same developmental period. Previous studies that have documented the association between childhood stress and TL have relied on adult measures of TL and retrospective recall of stress years after the stress was experienced.20, 21, 22, 23 In cross-sectional studies with a single measurement point for TL, it has not been possible to assess the association between stress exposure and change in TL over time. Moreover, given the elapsed time between the putative stress exposure and the measurement of TL, it has not been clear whether telomeres began eroding during stress exposure or whether erosion occurred years later, possibly promoted by the sequelae of childhood stress or other intervening variables. For example, given that maltreated children often grow up to be in poor physical health as adults52,53 telomere erosion could be a consequence of later health problems, as opposed to a proximal effect of maltreatment itself. This study provides evidence that childhood stress is related to telomere erosion over time, and while children are experiencing stress.

The results of this study underscore some of the complexities involved in research on psychosocial stress and telomere erosion. First, although each kind of violence exposure was associated with telomere erosion in the predicted direction, the most significant effect was observed between multiple violence exposures and telomere erosion. These findings are consistent with theory that the effect of stress is seen most clearly when stress is measured in a cumulative way.8, 33 As more studies accumulate, it will be possible to interrogate the specific features of a stressor that matter most in relation to telomere erosion (for example, duration, severity, physical harm, perceived threat). Second, some of the complexities revealed in recent reports using repeated measurements of TL were confirmed here, including that in some individuals telomeres can lengthen.26, 27, 28, 29, 30, 31Interestingly, most ‘lengtheners’ in our and others’ studies have shorter TL at baseline.26, 28, 31 Observed lengthening could result from (a) measurement error, (b) regression to the mean, (c) differences in cells sampled at different ages (even if the tissue that is sampled is held constant, as in our study) or (d) meaningful telomere dynamics involving telomerase activity or alternative lengthening of telomeres in cells that lack telomerase activity.54, 55 As TL can predict health outcomes, the mechanisms that regulate TL are of great interest and demand more investigation.

The results of this study raise the question of what biological processes explain the association between psychosocial stress and telomere erosion. Most of the insights about mechanisms associated with telomere erosion originate from research on oxidative stress and inflammation, indicating both as important influences on TL.56, 57, 58, 59 Telomeres are sensitive to damage by oxidative stress, as demonstrated by experiments showing increased erosion under conditions of high reactive oxygen species in vitro.56 Inflammation is associated with increased proliferation of immune cells and, as a consequence, with more telomere erosion.60 Childhood maltreatment predicts elevated inflammation,61suggesting a possible cause for the increased telomere erosion observed in this study. A study of Alzheimer's disease patients’ caregivers demonstrated association between TL and altered T-cell function, as well as immune markers.62Research is needed to test whether effects of psychosocial stress on telomere erosion are mediated by oxidative stress and immune-system changes in children.

This study has several limitations. First, there are tradeoffs regarding the best ways to measure TL. The two main methods are southern blot analysis of the terminal restriction fragments63 and quantitative reverse transcriptase-PCR to measure the ratio between a single-copy gene and telomeric repeat region (T/Sratio).41 The T/S ratio was used in this study. Its principal advantage is that it can be performed at high-throughput and low-cost. However, the T/S ratio can suffer measurement error; prior studies denote a CV range between 0.9 and 28%. In this study, we observed low measurement error; CV was <1% in all assays. Moreover, measurement errors should primarily affect assays among individuals with a slower rate of telomere erosion,26, 27, 28 whereas slower erosion is more common in adults than children.32

A second limitation concerns the measurement of TL in different tissues.64 As a result of ethical difficulties obtaining blood from children in the community, this study and two others of children have used buccal cells,25, 51 instead of the peripheral blood cells more commonly used in studies of adults. Buccal swabs may not only yield buccal cells, and it is possible that infiltration of immune cells because of poor oral hygiene or infection could alter oral cell composition, which has different telomere dynamics than buccal cells. Positive correlations have been reported between TL from buccal cells and TL from blood.65 In addition, buccal cell TL has been associated with age-related disease,66 although one report showed no significant correlation between TL from buccal cells and white blood cells.67 Our and others’ studies suggest that estimating TL from buccal cells can detect effects of stress, although validation in larger studies that measure TL from multiple tissues is needed.

Third, our sample comprised MZ twins. However, to our knowledge there is no evidence that MZ twins react differently to violence or have different TL dynamics than singletons. Unfortunately, we were unable to compare TL in twin siblings discordant for violence exposure, as there were too few discordant pairs for adequate statistical power. We intend to attempt a discordant twin comparison in our larger cohort in the future. Fourth, our first TL measure was taken at age-5 years, but some mothers reported their children experienced violence before age-5 as well as after. Figure 2 hints at this possibility by showing that children with violence exposure already had shorter TL at age-5 than children without violence exposure. Nevertheless, children with multiple violence exposures showed subsequent accelerated erosion between ages 5 to 10 years.

In conclusion, our findings provide evidence that stress-related accelerated telomere erosion can be observed already in childhood. This suggests the importance of integrating telomeres as stress markers in research to evaluate the effects of maltreatment on young victims. TL measurement is now offered to adults as a diagnostic tool to monitor health and predict disease risk.68 It is conceivable that research may eventually implicate TL measurement in clinical pediatrics. However, extreme caution should be taken as more research is needed to uncover mechanisms that govern TL dynamics. Meanwhile, our findings help to address the basic-science puzzle of how and when childhood stress gets ‘under the skin’ at the cellular level.

Top

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Top

References

  1. Wegman HL, Stetler C. A meta-analytic review of the effects of childhood abuse on medical outcomes in adulthood. Psychosom Med 2009; 71: 805–812. | Article | PubMed |
  2. Rich-Edwards JW, Spiegelman D, Lividoti Hibert EN, Jun HJ, Todd TJ, Kawachi I et al. Abuse in childhood and adolescence as a predictor of type 2 diabetes in adult women. Am J Prev Med 2010; 39: 529–536. | Article | PubMed |
  3. Riley EH, Wright RJ, Jun HJ, Hibert EN, Rich-Edwards JW. Hypertension in adult survivors of child abuse: observations from the Nurses’ Health Study II. J Epidemiol Community Health 2010; 64: 413–418. | Article | PubMed |
  4. Dong M, Giles WH, Felitti VJ, Dube SR, Williams JE, Chapman DP et al. Insights into causal pathways for ischemic heart disease: adverse childhood experiences study. Circulation 2004; 110: 1761–1766. | Article | PubMed |
  5. Nanni V, Uher R, Danese A. Childhood maltreatment predicts unfavorable course of illness and treatment outcome in depression: a meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry 2012; 169: 141–151. | Article | PubMed |
  6. Miller GE, Chen E, Parker KJ. Psychological stress in childhood and susceptibility to the chronic diseases of aging: moving toward a model of behavioral and biological mechanisms. Psychol Bull 2011; 137: 959–997. | Article | PubMed | ISI |
  7. Resilience in children. Proceedings of a conference sponsored by the New York Academy of Sciences and Brown Medical School. Arlington, VA, USA.Ann NY Acad Sci 2006; 1094: 1–368. | Article | PubMed |
  8. Danese A, McEwen BS. Adverse childhood experiences, allostasis, allostatic load, and age-related disease. Physiol Behav 2012; 106: 29–39. | Article | PubMed |
  9. Epel ES. Telomeres in a life-span perspective: a new ″psychobiomarker″?Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2009; 18: 6–10. | Article |
  10. Hayflick L, Moorhead PS. The serial cultivation of human diploid cell strains.Exp Cell Res 1961; 25: 585–621. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  11. Olovnikov AM. A theory of marginotomy. The incomplete copying of template margin in enzymic synthesis of polynucleotides and biological significance of the phenomenon. J Theor Biol 1973; 41: 181–190. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  12. Harley CB, Futcher AB, Greider CW. Telomeres shorten during ageing of human fibroblasts. Nature 1990; 345: 458–460. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  13. Blackburn EH, Gall JG. A tandemly repeated sequence at the termini of the extrachromosomal ribosomal RNA genes in Tetrahymena. J Mol Biol 1978;120: 33–53. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  14. Valdes AM, Andrew T, Gardner JP, Kimura M, Oelsner E, Cherkas LF et al. Obesity, cigarette smoking, and telomere length in women. Lancet 2005;366: 662–664. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  15. Buxton JL, Walters RG, Visvikis-Siest S, Meyre D, Froguel P, Blakemore AI. Childhood obesity is associated with shorter leukocyte telomere length. J Clin Endocrinol Metabol 2011; 96: 1500–1505. | Article |
  16. Nordfjall K, Eliasson M, Stegmayr B, Melander O, Nilsson P, Roos G. Telomere length is associated with obesity parameters but with a gender difference.Obesity (Silver Spring) 2008; 16: 2682–2689. | Article | PubMed | CAS |
  17. Yu WY, Chang HW, Lin CH, Cho CL. Short telomeres in patients with chronic schizophrenia who show a poor response to treatment. J Psychiatry Neurosci 2008; 33: 244–247. | PubMed |
  18. Simon NM, Smoller JW, McNamara KL, Maser RS, Zalta AK, Pollack MH et al. Telomere shortening and mood disorders: preliminary support for a chronic stress model of accelerated aging. Biol Psychiatry 2006; 60: 432–435. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  19. Epel ES, Blackburn EH, Lin J, Dhabhar FS, Adler NE, Morrow JD et al. Accelerated telomere shortening in response to life stress. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2004; 101: 17312–17315. | Article | PubMed | CAS |
  20. Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Gouin JP, Weng NP, Malarkey WB, Beversdorf DQ, Glaser R. Childhood adversity heightens the impact of later-life caregiving stress on telomere length and inflammation. Psychosom Med 2011; 73: 16–22. | Article | PubMed |
  21. Tyrka AR, Price LH, Kao HT, Porton B, Marsella SA, Carpenter LL. Childhood maltreatment and telomere shortening: preliminary support for an effect of early stress on cellular aging. Biol Psychiatry 2010; 67: 531–534. | Article | PubMed | ISI |
  22. Kananen L, Surakka I, Pirkola S, Suvisaari J, Lonnqvist J, Peltonen L et al. Childhood adversities are associated with shorter telomere length at adult age both in individuals with an anxiety disorder and controls. PloS One 2010;5: e10826. | Article | PubMed |
  23. O’Donovan A, Epel E, Lin J, Wolkowitz O, Cohen B, Maguen S et al. Childhood trauma associated with short leukocyte telomere length in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2011; 70: 465–471. | Article | PubMed |
  24. Glass D, Parts L, Knowles D, Aviv A, Spector TD. No correlation between childhood maltreatment and telomere length. Biol Psychiatry 2010; 68: e21–e22; author reply e23-24. | Article | PubMed |
  25. Drury SS, Theall K, Gleason MM, Smyke AT, De Vivo I, Wong JY et al. Telomere length and early severe social deprivation: linking early adversity and cellular aging. Mol Psychiatry; advance online publication, 17 May 2011; doi:10.1038/mp.2011.53. | Article |
  26. Aviv A, Chen W, Gardner JP, Kimura M, Brimacombe M, Cao X et al. Leukocyte telomere dynamics: longitudinal findings among young adults in the Bogalusa Heart Study. Am J Epidemiol 2009; 169: 323–329. | Article | PubMed | ISI |
  27. Chen W, Kimura M, Kim S, Cao X, Srinivasan SR, Berenson GS et al. Longitudinal versus cross-sectional evaluations of leukocyte telomere length dynamics: age-dependent telomere shortening is the rule. J Gerontol Ser A Biol Sci Med Sci 2011; 66: 312–319. | Article |
  28. Ehrlenbach S, Willeit P, Kiechl S, Willeit J, Reindl M, Schanda K et al. Influences on the reduction of relative telomere length over 10 years in the population-based Bruneck Study: introduction of a well-controlled high-throughput assay. Int J Epidemiol 2009; 38: 1725–1734. | Article | PubMed |
  29. Epel ES, Merkin SS, Cawthon R, Blackburn EH, Adler NE, Pletcher MJ et al. The rate of leukocyte telomere shortening predicts mortality from cardiovascular disease in elderly men. Aging 2009; 1: 81–88. | ISI | CAS |
  30. Farzaneh-Far R, Lin J, Epel E, Lapham K, Blackburn E, Whooley MA. Telomere length trajectory and its determinants in persons with coronary artery disease: longitudinal findings from the heart and soul study. PloS One 2010;5: e8612. | Article | PubMed | CAS |
  31. Nordfjall K, Svenson U, Norrback KF, Adolfsson R, Lenner P, Roos G. The individual blood cell telomere attrition rate is telomere length dependent.PLoS Genet 2009; 5: e1000375. | Article | PubMed |
  32. Zeichner SL, Palumbo P, Feng Y, Xiao X, Gee D, Sleasman J et al. Rapid telomere shortening in children. Blood 1999; 93: 2824–2830. | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  33. Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, Williamson DF, Spitz AM, Edwards V et al. Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults. The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. Am J Prev Med 1998; 14: 245–258. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  34. Trouton A, Spinath FM, Plomin R. Twins early development study (TEDS): a multivariate, longitudinal genetic investigation of language, cognition and behavior problems in childhood. Twin Res Off J In Soc Twin Stud 2002; 5: 444–448. | Article |
  35. Moffitt TE. Teen-aged mothers in contemporary Britain. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Disciplines 2002; 43: 727–742. | Article |
  36. Ouellet-Morin I, Odgers CL, Danese A, Bowes L, Shakoor S, Papadopoulos AS et al. Blunted cortisol responses to stress signal social and behavioral problems among maltreated/bullied 12-year-old children. Biol Psychiatry2011; 70: 1016–1023. | Article | PubMed |
  37. Danese A, Caspi A, Williams B, Ambler A, Sugden K, Mika J et al. Biological embedding of stress through inflammation processes in childhood. Mol Psychiatry 2011; 16: 244–246. | Article | PubMed |
  38. Bowtell DD. Rapid isolation of eukaryotic DNA. Anal Biochem 1987; 162: 463–465. | Article | PubMed |
  39. Jeanpierre M. A rapid method for the purification of DNA from blood. Nucleic Acids Res 1987; 15: 9611. | Article | PubMed | CAS |
  40. Freeman B, Smith N, Curtis C, Huckett L, Mill J, Craig IW. DNA from buccal swabs recruited by mail: evaluation of storage effects on long-term stability and suitability for multiplex polymerase chain reaction genotyping. Behav Genet 2003; 33: 67–72. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  41. Cawthon RM. Telomere measurement by quantitative PCR. Nucleic Acids Res2002; 30: e47. | Article | PubMed |
  42. Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Krueger RF, Magdol L, Margolin G, Silva PA et al. Do partners agree about abuse in their relationship? A psychometric evaluation of interpartner agreement. Psychol Assessment 1997; 9: 47–56. | Article |
  43. Arseneault L, Walsh E, Trzesniewski K, Newcombe R, Caspi A, Moffitt TE. Bullying victimization uniquely contributes to adjustment problems in young children: a nationally representative cohort study. Pediatrics 2006; 118: 130–138. | Article | PubMed |
  44. Shakoor S, Jaffee SR, Andreou P, Bowes L, Ambler AP, Caspi A et al. Mothers and children as informants of bullying victimization: results from an epidemiological cohort of children. J Abnormal Child Psychol 2011; 39: 379–387. | Article |
  45. Jaffee SR, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Taylor A. Physical maltreatment victim to antisocial child: evidence of an environmentally mediated process. J Abnormal Psychol 2004; 113: 44–55. | Article |
  46. Jaffee SR, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Polo-Tomas M, Price TS, Taylor A. The limits of child effects: evidence for genetically mediated child effects on corporal punishment but not on physical maltreatment. Dev Psychol 2004; 40: 1047–1058. | Article | PubMed |
  47. Jaffee SR, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Polo-Tomas M, Taylor A. Individual, family, and neighborhood factors distinguish resilient from non-resilient maltreated children: a cumulative stressors model. Child Abuse Neglect 2007; 31: 231–253. | Article | PubMed |
  48. Straus MA, Hamby SL, Finkelhor D, Moore DW, Runyan D. Identification of child maltreatment with the Parent-Child Conflict Tactics Scales: development and psychometric data for a national sample of American parents. Child Abuse Neglect 1998; 22: 249–270. | Article | PubMed |
  49. Dodge KA, Bates JE, Pettit GS. Mechanisms in the cycle of violence. Science1990; 250: 1678–1683. | Article | PubMed | ISI |
  50. Williams RL. A note on robust variance estimation for cluster-correlated data. Biometrics 2000; 56: 645–646. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  51. Kroenke CH, Epel E, Adler N, Bush NR, Obradovic J, Lin J et al. Autonomic and adrenocortical reactivity and buccal cell telomere length in kindergarten children. Psychosom Med 2011; 73: 533–540. | Article | PubMed |
  52. Danese A, Moffitt TE, Harrington H, Milne BJ, Polanczyk G, Pariante CM et al. Adverse childhood experiences and adult risk factors for age-related disease: depression, inflammation, and clustering of metabolic risk markers.Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2009; 163: 1135–1143. | Article | PubMed | ISI |
  53. Springer KW, Sheridan J, Kuo D, Carnes M. Long-term physical and mental health consequences of childhood physical abuse: results from a large population-based sample of men and women. Child Abuse Neglect 2007; 31: 517–530. | Article | PubMed |
  54. Slatter TL, Tan X, Yuen YC, Gunningham S, Ma SS, Daly E et al. The alternative lengthening of telomeres pathway may operate in non-neoplastic human cells. J Pathol 2012; 226: 509–518. | Article | PubMed |
  55. Svenson U, Nordfjall K, Baird D, Roger L, Osterman P, Hellenius ML et al. Blood cell telomere length is a dynamic feature. PloS One 2011; 6: e21485. | Article | PubMed | CAS |
  56. von Zglinicki T. Oxidative stress shortens telomeres. Trends Biochem Sci2002; 27: 339–344. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  57. O’Donovan A, Pantell MS, Puterman E, Dhabhar FS, Blackburn EH, Yaffe K et al. Cumulative inflammatory load is associated with short leukocyte telomere length in the Health, Aging and Body Composition Study. PloS One 2011; 6: e19687. | Article | PubMed | CAS |
  58. Shiels PG, McGlynn LM, MacIntyre A, Johnson PC, Batty GD, Burns H et al. Accelerated telomere attrition is associated with relative household income, diet and inflammation in the pSoBid cohort. PloS One 2011; 6: e22521. | Article | PubMed |
  59. Ilmonen P, Kotrschal A, Penn DJ. Telomere attrition due to infection. PloS One 2008; 3: e2143. | Article | PubMed |
  60. Goronzy JJ, Fujii H, Weyand CM. Telomeres, immune aging and autoimmunity. Exp Gerontol 2006; 41: 246–251. | Article | PubMed |
  61. Danese A, Pariante CM, Caspi A, Taylor A, Poulton R. Childhood maltreatment predicts adult inflammation in a life-course study. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2007; 104: 1319–1324. | Article | PubMed | CAS |
  62. Damjanovic AK, Yang Y, Glaser R, Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Nguyen H, Laskowski Bet al. Accelerated telomere erosion is associated with a declining immune function of caregivers of Alzheimer's disease patients. J Immunol 2007;179: 4249–4254. | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  63. Allshire RC, Dempster M, Hastie ND. Human telomeres contain at least three types of G-rich repeat distributed non-randomly. Nucleic Acids Res 1989;17: 4611–4627. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  64. Hewakapuge S, van Oorschot RA, Lewandowski P, Baindur-Hudson S. Investigation of telomere lengths measurement by quantitative real-time PCR to predict age. Leg Med (Tokyo) 2008; 10: 236–242. | Article | PubMed |
  65. Gadalla SM, Cawthon R, Giri N, Alter BP, Savage SA. Telomere length in blood, buccal cells, and fibroblasts from patients with inherited bone marrow failure syndromes. Aging 2010; 2: 867–874. | PubMed |
  66. Broberg K, Bjork J, Paulsson K, Hoglund M, Albin M. Constitutional short telomeres are strong genetic susceptibility markers for bladder cancer.Carcinogenesis 2005; 26: 1263–1271. | Article | PubMed | CAS |
  67. Thomas P, NJ OC, Fenech M. Telomere length in white blood cells, buccal cells and brain tissue and its variation with ageing and Alzheimer's disease.Mech Ageing Dev 2008; 129: 183–190. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  68. Wolinsky H. Testing time for telomeres. Telomere length can tell us something about disease susceptibility and ageing, but are commercial tests ready for prime time? EMBO Rep 2011; 12: 897–900. | Article | PubMed |
Top

Acknowledgements

The first author is supported by NICHD Grant HD061298 and by the Jacobs Foundation. Dr Andrea Danese was supported by the 2009 NARSAD/Brain and Behaviour Research Foundation Young Investigator Award. The E-Risk Study is funded by the Medical Research Council (UKMRC Grants G1002190 and G9806489). Additional support was provided by NIA Grant AG032282, ESRC Grant RES-177-25-0013, NICHD Grant HD061298, and by funds from the Jacobs Foundation, the British Academy, the Nuffield Foundation and NIMH Grant MH077874. We are grateful to the study mothers, fathers and the twins for their participation, and to members of the E-Risk team for their dedication, hard work and insights.

Supplementary Information accompanies the paper on the Molecular Psychiatry website


Artigo original:

http://www.nature.com/mp/journal/v18/n5/full/mp201232a.html

O portal Psiquiatria Infantil.com.br já recebeu

47.745.759 visitantes