Artigos Científicos

Predicting aggression in children with ADHD

Elif Ercan; et al

2 de junho de 2014

Predicting aggression in children with ADHD

Elif Ercan1Eyüp Sabri Ercan2*, Hakan Atılgan3Bürge Kabukçu Başay2Taciser Uysal2Sevim Berrin İnci4 and Ülkü Akyol Ardıç5

Author Affiliations

1Department of Psychological Counseling and Guidance, Ege University Faculty of Education, Izmir, Turkey

2Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Ege University Faculty of Medicine, Izmir, Turkey

3Department of Educational Sciences Measurement and Evaluation in Education, Ege University Faculty of Education, Izmir, Turkey

4Ege University Institute on Drug Abuse, Toxicology and Pharmaceutical Science, İzmir, Turkey

5Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Denizli, Turkey

For all author emails, please log on.

Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health 2014, 8:15  doi:10.1186/1753-2000-8-15


© 2014 Ercan et al.; licensee BioMed Central Ltd. 

 

Abstract

Objective

The present study uses structural equation modeling of latent traits to examine the extent to which family factors, cognitive factors and perceptions of rejection in mother-child relations differentially correlate with aggression at home and at school.

Methods

Data were collected from 476 school-age (7–15 years old) children with a diagnosis of ADHD who had previously shown different types of aggressive behavior, as well as from their parents and teachers. Structural equation modeling was used to examine the differential relationships between maternal rejection, family, cognitive factors and aggression in home and school settings.

Results

Family factors influenced aggression reported at home (.68) and at school (.44); maternal rejection seems to be related to aggression at home (.21). Cognitive factors influenced aggression reported at school (.-05) and at home (-.12).

Conclusions

Both genetic and environmental factors contribute to the development of aggressive behavior in ADHD. Identifying key risk factors will advance the development of appropriate clinical interventions and prevention strategies and will provide information to guide the targeting of resources to those children at highest risk.

Keywords: 

Aggression; ADHD; Structural equation modeling

Background

ADHD is one of the most prevalent childhood disorders, and it is a community health problem that may result in significant psychiatric, social and academic problems if not treated. ADHD frequently co-occurs with other psychiatric disorders [1,2]. Research shows that aggression is an important associated feature of ADHD, and it is essential in understanding the impact of the disorder and its treatment [3]. The presence of comorbid aggression in ADHD does not appear to be spurious, and the severity and/or presence of aggression and ADHD may significantly impact its long-term prognosis. The etiology of aggression in ADHD is not clearly understood. However, aggression can be considered to be an outcome of the interaction between genetic and environmental factors [4]. Aggression is thought to be inherited, and the concordance of maternal twins is between .28 and .72 [5]. Compared to children who only have ADHD, it is more likely that children with ADHD and ODD or CD have fathers with an Antisocial Personality Disorder. Pfiffner et al. [6] found that children who have fathers with Antisocial Personality Disorder are more at risk for developing behavioral problems.

The most significant family factors influencing the occurrence of aggression in ADHD are as follows: large family size, the attitude of the family towards aggression, disciplinary or negative parenting, low socio economic status and family conflict [7]. Extended family and low socio economic status may cause aggression as a result of inadequate attention.

Parental attitudes are particularly important in psychiatric disorders, including aggression and ADHD [8]. However, there is a gap in the literature regarding the nature of the relationship between negative parental attitudes and psychiatric disorders that influence childhood aggression. The debate over whether aggression in children caused by parents’ lack of interest and/or their hostile and critical attitudes towards their children, or whether negative parenting is instead caused by children’s behavioral problems remains unresolved [9].

Cognitive deficits primarily in the verbal area play a role in the etiology of aggression. Previous data regarding the interaction between cognition and aggression reveal such general cognitive predictors of aggression as lower intelligence quotients, reading difficulties, and problems associated with attention and hyperactivity [10]. Many studies suggest that aggressive children experience problems in social cognitive areas [11,12] and have lower IQ scores [13,14]. In a meta-analysis of twenty-seven studies, seventeen studies reported negative associations between cognitive functions and disruptive behaviors [15].

Some of the most comprehensive research examining the relationship between ADHD and aggression using advanced statistical analyses has been conducted by Miller et al. [16]. In that study, 165 children with ADHD and disruptive behaviors between the ages of 7 and 11 were tested using structural equation modeling (SEM) to determine the influence of family and cognitive factors on aggression. One of the most important characteristics of the study is that it attempts to explain aggression in children with ADHD with information from two sources: parents and teachers. Family factors including present and past aggression by parents and the number of siblings are examined. Cognitive factors, verbal IQ, reading and mathematical achievement are also examined. The study found that family factors are related to aggression at home and at school, whereas cognitive factors are only related to aggression at school.

The purpose of our study is to evaluate the influence of family, parent–child relations and cognitive factors on the development of aggression in children within a larger and a non-western sample. We use structural equation modeling and include information from the parents, teachers and the child as the information source. This method is ideal, as it is important to receive information from multiple sources to explain a multicomponent concept such as aggression. Accordingly, we include evaluations of the mothers’ acceptance or rejection of the child with ADHD in the structural equation model in addition to information received from parents and teachers. To our knowledge, this is the first study to consider information from the parent, teacher and the child regarding aggression in ADHD. In addition, we examine mother-child relationships in detail regarding the etiology of aggression [8,16], as we consider it crucial to include the perception of acceptance or rejection of children with ADHD by their mothers as a possible latent factor.

In our study, past and current aggression by the parents, the number of people living in the home and the number of siblings were used as family factors. To define cognitive factors in the present study, verbal and performance IQ and school success variables are used. To evaluate the perceptions of children regarding their mothers’ acceptance or rejection, warmth, aggression and rejection variables specified in the theory of parental acceptance and rejection are used [17].

Methods

Diagnosis of ADHD

In total, 476 subjects referred to the Disruptive Behavior Disorders Clinic in 2011 with a diagnosis of ADHD with aggressive behaviors were included in the study, in addition to their parents and teachers. Approval from The Institutional Review Board (IRB) at the Ege University School of Medicine was attained before the study began, and informed consent was gathered from the parents.

Our recruitment and screening procedures were designed to collect data from a carefully diagnosed sample of children for ADHD comorbidities and subtypes. The children were first interviewed by a senior child psychiatry resident using the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School Age Children: Present and Lifetime version (K-SADS-PL) [18]. The K-SADS-PL is a highly reliable semi-structured interview for the assessment of a wide range of psychiatric disorders. Cognitive assessments were performed using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised (WISC-R) [19]. Subjects with an IQ less than 70 were excluded from the study. Those who met the inclusion criteria for the study also completed the Children’s Aggression Scale-Parent and Teacher Versions (CAS-P, CAS-T), Teacher Report Form (TRF), Turgay DSM-IV Disruptive Behavior Disorders Rating Scale (T-DSM-IV-S) parent and teacher forms, and the Parental Acceptance and Rejection Questionnaire (PARQ), completed by both the parents and teachers of the participants.

The returned parent and teacher version of T-DSM-IV-S forms were scored, and the children who scored less than one standard deviation below the relevant age norms on the Attention Deficiency and Hyperactivity Disorder subscales were excluded from the study. The T-DSM-IV-S was developed by Turgay [20] and translated and adapted by Ercan, Amado, Somer, & Cikoglu [21]. The T-DSM-IV-S is based on DSM-IV diagnostic criteria and assesses hyperactivity-impulsivity (9 items), inattention (9 items), opposition-defiance (8 items), and conduct disorder (15 items). Symptoms are scored by assigning a severity estimate for each symptom on a 4-point Likert scale (0 = not at all; 1 = just a little; 2 = quite a bit; and 3 = very much). The subscale scores on the T-DSM-IV-S were calculated by summing the scores on the items of each subscale. Similar scales derived from the DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for AD/HD, such as the AD/HD Rating Scale IV, have been shown to have adequate criterion-related validity and good reliability in different cultures both by parents and teachers [22,23]. The second diagnostic interview was conducted by an experienced child psychiatrist who knew that the child was a candidate for the study but was blind to the first judge’s diagnosis of comorbid disorders and ADHD subtypes. “A best estimate procedure” was used to determine the final diagnoses. “Best estimate procedure” is defined here as determining the diagnostic status after reviewing all teacher and parent scales and the K-SADS-PL, and WISC-R results.

Dependent variables of the study

This study has two main dependent measures: aggression at home and aggression at school in elementary school students with ADHD.

Children’s aggression scale – parent & teacher forms (CAS-P & CAS-T)

These scales were designed by Halperin et al. [24,25]. Both the 33-item CAS–P and 23-item CAS–T require informants to indicate the frequency (i.e., never, once per month or less, once per week or less, 2–3 times per week, or most days) with which the child has engaged in various aggressive behaviors during the past year. The CAS–P was entered into the model to indicate aggression in the home, and the CAS–T was entered to indicate aggression in school settings. Each test has five separate subscales: verbal aggression, aggression against objects and animals, provoked physical aggression, initiated physical aggression, and the use of weapons.

Independent variables of the study

This study includes three independent measures of familial risk factors, cognitive risk factors, and children’s perceptions of acceptance and rejection in their relationships with their mothers.

Familial risk factors were evaluated by interview. A child psychiatrist asked the parents about the number of siblings, the number of people living in the home, and the parents’ present and past history of aggression. The Teacher Report Form (TRF) was used to obtain the children’s academic performance, and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised (WISC-R) was used to assess cognitive risk factors. The “Parental Acceptance/Rejection Questionnaire (PARQ)” was used to determine the children’s perceptions of their acceptance/rejection by their mothers.

The Parental Acceptance/Rejection Questionnaire (PARQ)

This scale was designed by Rohner, Saavedra and Granum in 1978 to assess the perceived acceptance/rejection of children with respect to their relationships with their parents. The PARQ includes four sub-scales: “Warmth (20 items), Hostility/Aggression (15 items), Neglect and Indifference (15 items), and Undifferentiated Rejection (10 items)”. The total scores for these sub-scales reflect the degree of perception, with higher scores indicating perceived rejection.

Teacher Report Form (TRF)

The Teacher Report Form (TRF) was developed by Achenbach and Edelbrock [26] and adapted by Erol, Arslan, & Akçakın [27]. The Turkish Form of the TRF is normed for children 4–18 years of age and provides reliable and valid measures of the children’s school adaptation and problematic behaviors.

Statistical methodology

In the first part of the data analysis, we used IBM PASW Statistics 18 for descriptive statistical analyses, and the data were presented as means (standard deviations), percentages, medians, and minimum and maximum values, where appropriate. In the second part, we used SPSS AMOS 18 for testing the structural equation model.

Results

In total, 476 subjects between 7 and 15 years of age (±2.11) diagnosed with ADHD were included in the study. The majority (79% of participants; n = 376) were boys, and 21% (n = 100) were girls. The distribution of diagnostic groups and their percentages in the study population are presented in Table 1. The cases were diagnosed as “pure” ADHD (37.8%), ADHD + ODD (44.3%) and ADHD + CD (17.9%). Descriptive statistics for the observed variables in the SEM hypothesis are presented in Table 2.

Table 1. Diagnoses of participants and their percentages in the study population(N = 476)

Table 2. Descriptive statistics of observed variables in the SEM hypothesis (N = 476)

SEM analysis of our proposed model consisted of two separate elements, of which the first is a measurement model (confirmatory factor analysis-CFA) and the second is a structural model (Figure 1).

thumbnailFigure 1. Structural equation modeling of aggression in elementary school students with ADHD (standardized solution;N=476; *: p<0.05, **: p<0.001).

Measurement model (confirmatory factor analysis)

The measurement model based upon a confirmatory factor analysis indicated that each of our measures was related to the latent variables with determination coefficients ranging from .92 to .01. Standardized and unstandardized regression weights, determination coefficients, and significance levels of these variables are shown in Table 3.

Table 3. Unstandardized estimatesstandardized estimatesdetermination coefficientsand significance levels for model in Figure 1 (N = 476)

Categorical variables

The dichotomous variables of our data were fathers’ or mothers’ presence of aggression whether at present or at past. Until recently, two primary approaches to the analysis of categorical data[28,29] have dominated this area of research. Both methodologies use standard estimates of polychoric and polyserial correlations, followed by a type of asymptotic distribution-free (ADF) methodology for the structured model. However, because of the ultra-restrictive assumptions of these methodologies, they are impractical and difficult to meet.

AMOS software uses Bayesian estimation (BE) method for categorical data via an algorithm termed the Markov Chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) algorithm.

Our data isn’t normally distributed so to estimate the parameters, the model is put in a Bayesian framework. After BE procedure we treated our categorical variables with a maximum likelihood (ML) procedure. The BE and ML procedures showed similar results with minimal or no differences. The comparisons of BE and ML results are shown in Table 4.

Table 4. Comparison of factor loading unstandardized parameter estimates:maximum likelihood versus Bayesian estimation

Structural model

In the second part of SEM analysis, we calculated estimates of the relationships, and we tested our model for fit. The structural model analysis in our study revealed statistically significant cross-loadings of aggression at home and aggression at school with the perception of acceptance/rejection by the mothers, family factors, and cognitive factors (Figure 2). There was a non-significant loading of the Perception of Acceptance or Rejection in Parent Relationships on aggression at school. The standardized and unstandardized regression weights and the significance levels of these variables are shown in Table 3.

thumbnailFigure 2. Structural equation modeling of aggression in elementary school students with ADHD (standardized solution;N=476; *: p<0.05, **: p<0.001).

Testing the model-fit

The χ2 value of our model was 249.199, which is a large value. The Likelihood Ratio Test of the null hypothesis (H0) of this χ2 value revealed a non-significant probability, p = .11. As the χ2probability of .11 was non-significant (p > .05), our model fit the data well.

The χ2 value of our model was 249.199, which is a large value. Because the χ2 statistic equals (N–1) Fmin, which means sample size minus 1, multiplied by the minimum fit function, this value tends to be substantial when the model does not hold and when sample size is large [30]. When our sample size, which is large enough, is considered, a higher χ2 value does make sense. The Likelihood Ratio Test results of the null hypothesis (H0) of this χ2 value revealed a non-significant probability, p = 0.11. The probability value associated with χ2 represents the likelihood of obtaining a χ2 value that exceeds the χ2 value when H0 is true. Thus, the higher the probability associated with χ2, the closer the fit between the hypothesized model (under H0) and the perfect fit [31]. As of our probability of 0.11 reveals (p > 0.05, non-significant), our model can be defined as a well-fitted model.

We used the CMIN/DF value as a second measure to test the fit of our model. Values of CMIN/DF lower than 2 indicate an acceptable fit [32-34], and our model fulfilled this criterion (CMIN/DF = 1.117).

The NFI value was .906, and the CFI value was .989 as shown in Table 3. The NFI value suggested that the model fit was only marginally adequate (NFI: .906), yet acceptable, but the CFI value suggests a superior fit (CFI: .989). The Incremental Index of Fit (IFI) [35] was developed to address issues of parsimony and sample size, which are known to be associated with the NFI. Unsurprisingly, our IFI of .989 is more consistent with the CFI and reflects a well-fitting model. Finally, the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) [36], consistent with the other indices noted here, yielded values ranging from zero to 1.00, with values close to .95 (for large samples) being indicative of good fit [37]. As shown in Table 3, our TLI value of .986 is indicative of a superior fit of our model.

The final index was the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA). This index was one of the most informative criteria in covariance structure modeling. The RMSEA takes into account the error of approximation in the population and asks the question “How well would the model, with unknown but optimally chosen parameter values, fit the population covariance matrix if it were available?” [38]. This discrepancy, as measured by the RMSEA, is expressed per degree of freedom, thus making it sensitive to the number of estimated parameters in the model (i.e., the complexity of the model); values less than .05 indicate good fit. The RMSEA value in our model was .019 as shown in Table 3, which represents a good fit.

When all of the indices are considered, we conclude that the proposed model fits our data well. The child’s perception of acceptance/rejection by the mothers significantly predicts aggression at home (β = .21, p = .012), whereas this perception does not predict aggression at school (p = .238). Family factors significantly predict aggression at home (β = .68, p < .001), and aggression at school (β = .44, p < .001). Likewise, cognitive factors significantly predict aggression at home (β = -.12, p = .032) and aggression at school (β = -.05, p = .028).

When all predictors of aggression levels are considered together, they predict 52% of the variance in overall aggression at home and 20% of the overall variance in aggression at school.

Discussion

Even though aggressive behavior in children with ADHD is highly prevalent, it is not well understood [3]. Despite the existing literature on the influence of family factors, cognitive function and parent–child relationship problems on aggression in ADHD, there are few studies concerning the relationships of these factors with aggression at home and school. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study examining the influence of family, cognitive and maternal acceptance or rejection factors on school-age children with ADHD with a large sample and using structural equation modeling.

The most important finding from this study is that family is the most important factor in predicting aggression in children with ADHD both at school and at home. This finding is in accordance with the findings of Miller et al. [16], which also model factors relating to aggression in ADHD with similar methodologies and statistics [16]. In both studies, family factors are found to be the most important factors in aggression both at school and at home. In our study, parents’ past and present aggression, the number of siblings and the number of people living in the same home are also evaluated as potential family indicators. We find that the number of siblings and the number of people living in the home do not significantly predict aggression at school or at home. Parents’ past and present aggression is the most important variable for predicting the aggression of children at school and at home. This finding is consistent with previous research, which clearly suggests that parents’ antisocial behavior is strongly and specifically related to their children’s aggressive behavior [39]. Although it is difficult to parse out the genetic and environmental influences, it is likely that aggressive parents play an important role in the emergence and persistence of aggression in children. For example, one study indicates that the more the aggressive parent is absent from the home, the smaller the effect that parent’s behavior has on the behavior of the children in the home [40]. Even if the genetic contribution of parents’ aggressive behavior is controlled, parental aggression nonetheless affects the child’s aggressive behaviors [41]. These findings in these studies support the importance of modeling environmental effects.

In our study, we evaluated the perceptions of children with ADHD regarding their acceptance or rejection by their mothers. The child’s perception of acceptance of rejection by the mothers is only related to aggression at home and not to aggression at school. In addition, we found that family factors predict aggression at home more than acceptance or rejection by the mother.

This finding suggests that the relationship between parenting and children’s behavior may be more complicated than previously thought, though it is in accordance with other studies of the influence of maternal attitudes on childhood aggression. In contrast with these previous studies, recent studies show that the correlation between parenting and children’s behavioral problems may not be linear. Yeh, Chen, Raine, Bakre, & Jacobson [42] find that the correlation between parenting and children’s behavioral problems depends upon the intensity of the children’s behavioral problems. In other words, similar parental attitudes may have different influences on different children. Cartwright et al. [43] also found that negative maternal emotions expressed towards children with ADHD (e.g., low warmth and hostility/criticism) are more damaging than emotions expressed towards children without ADHD. In this case, in addition to the impact of negative parenting on behavioral problems in children, it is important to also consider the influence of children’s behavioral problems on parents’ attitudes. In the study of Lifford et al. [44] a casual hypothesis of family relations influencing ADHD symptoms was not supported. Moreover, in many studies evaluating parental attitudes towards ADHD, parental attitudes improve after the administration of methylphenidate for the treatment of their children’s ADHD [45]. As a result of treatment, the resulting amelioration of the behavior may change the mother’s attitude towards the child. Based on these findings, the fact that maternal acceptance or rejection predicts childhood aggression only at home and is less predictive than other family factors suggests that parent–child relations have a secondary influence in cases of ADHD and that past and current parental aggression are the most important factors.

The third aim of our study was to evaluate the effects of cognitive factors on aggression in children with ADHD. Our findings reveal that children with lower cognitive function show more aggressive behaviors both at school and at home. This finding is consistent with many other studies in the literature, which also report that aggressive children have problems in social cognitive areas[10,11] and have lower IQ scores [12-14]. However, in our study, the correlation between cognitive factors and aggression at school and at home is less influential than family factors. This new information suggests that cognitive factors may have a limited scope of influence.

Limitations

The most important limitation of this study is its cross-sectional methodology. Longitudinal studies are needed to better assess aggression in cases of ADHD. In addition, this study was not able to evaluate whether aggression is relational or social. The fact that the family’s socioeconomic situation was not assessed in detail is another limitation of our study. Another limitation of our study is that maternal acceptance and rejection perceptions were assessed, but paternal acceptance and rejection perceptions were not assessed.

Clinical implications

ADHD is a prevalent psychiatric disorder, and it may cause significant complications if left untreated. The comorbidity of aggression has a negative influence on the treatment and prognosis of ADHD. In cases of ADHD comorbid with aggression, aggressive symptoms are more apparent and continuous compared to ADHD cases without aggression. Within this context, it is appropriate to evaluate ADHD cases first in terms of family factors, and then for cognitive and parent–child relational factors before the emergence of aggressive symptoms.

Key points

 

• What’s known: Past research has shown that when a child is referred with aggressive symptoms, one of the most common diagnoses is attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).

• What’s new: Previous studies have not examined which demographic factors, family factors, perception of acceptance/rejection by the mothers and cognitive factors differentially correlate with aggression at home and at school.

• Findings: Family factors, cognitive factors and perception of acceptance/rejection by the mothers are important aspects of ADHD children’s aggression.

• This study confirms that family factors affect aggressive behaviors of ADHD children at home and at school settings.

• Cognitive factors determine the aggressive behaviors of elementary school students’ aggression in both school and home.

• The child’s perception of acceptance of rejection by the mothers is related to aggression at home and not to aggression at school.

• Implications: Prevention and intervention programs that target aggressive behaviors of ADHD children by focusing on family factors, cognitive factors and perception of acceptance rejection by parents may have the most impact.

Competing interest

The study was not supported by any financial funding. No financial or material support was taken for the study. Dr. Ercan is on advisory boards for Eli Lilly Turkey and Janssen Turkey. The other authors have no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

Authors’ contributions

All authors but BKB contributed equally to the design and conduct of the study, interpretation of the results, and writing of the manuscript. BKB was responsible for collection of the data. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to (in alphabetical order) Ayse Er, Gunay Sagduyu and Semra Ucar for administration and scoring of the WISC-R. We are also thankful to children, parents and teachers who took part in this study.

References

  1. Connor DF, Steeber J, McBurnett K: A review of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder complicated by symptoms of oppositional defiant disorder or conduct disorder.

    J Dev and Behav Pediatrics 2010, 31:427-440. Publisher Full Text OpenURL

     

  2. Hinshaw SP, Park T: Research problems and issues: toward a more definitive science of distruptive behavior disorders. In The Handbook of Distruptive Behavior Disorders. Edited by Quay HC, Hogan AE. New York: NY: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers; 1999:593-620. OpenURL

     

  3. King S, Waschbusch DA: Aggression in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.

    Expert Rev Neurother 2010, 10:1581-1594. PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text OpenURL

     

  4. Retz W, Rözler M: The relation of ADHAD and violent aggression: what can we learn from epidemiological and genetic studies?

    Int J Law Psychiatr 2009, 32:235-243. Publisher Full Text OpenURL

     

  5. Hudziak JJ, van Beijsterveldt CE, Bartels M, Rietveld MJ, Rettew DC, Derks EM, Boomsma DI: Individual differences in aggression: genetic analyses by age, gender, and informant in 3-, 7-, and 10-year-old Dutch twins.

    Behav Genet 2003, 33:575-589. PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text OpenURL

     

  6. Pfiffner LJ, McBurnett K, Lahey BB, Loeber R, Green S, Frick PJ, Rathouz PJ: Association of parental psychopathology to the comorbid disorders of boys with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder.

    J Consult Clin Psychol 1999, 67:881-893. PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text OpenURL

     

  7. Haapasalo J, Tremblay RE: Physically aggressive boys from ages 6 to 12: family background, parenting behavior, and prediction of delinquency.

    J Consult Clin Psychol 1994, 62:1044-1052. PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text OpenURL

     

  8. Lahey BB: Out of the funhouse mirrors: steps toward understanding the role of parenting in maladaptive child development.

    J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2011, 50:975-977. PubMed Abstract |Publisher Full Text OpenURL

     

  9. Loober R, Hay D: Key issues in the development of aggression and violence from childhood to early adulthood.

    Annu Rev Psychol 1997, 48:371-410. PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text OpenURL

     

  10. Crick NR, Dodge KA: A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment.

    Psycol Bull 1994, 115:74-101. OpenURL

     

  11. Dodge KA: A social information processing model of social competence in children. In Minnesota Symposium in Child Psychology. Edited by Perlmutter M. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1986:77-125. OpenURL

     

  12. Heilburn AD Jr: The measurement of criminal dangerousness as a personality construct: further validation of a research index.

    J Pers Assess 1990, 54:141-148. PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text OpenURL

     

  13. Myers WC, Scott K, Burgess AW, Burgess AG: Psychopathology, biopsychosocial factors, crime characteristics, and classification of 25 homicidal youths.

    J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1995, 34:1483-1489. PubMed Abstract |Publisher Full Text OpenURL

     

  14. Pineda DA, Roselli M, Henao GC, Mejia SE: Neurobehavioral assessment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in a Colombian sample.

    Appl Neuropsychol 2000, 7:40-46. PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text OpenURL

     

  15. Loeber R, Slot NW: Stouthamer-Loeber M: A cumulative developmental model of risk and promotive factors. In Tomorrow’s criminals: The development of child delinquency and effective interventions. Edited by Loeber R, Slot NW, Laan PH, Hoeve M. Farnham: Ashgate; 2008:133-161. OpenURL

     

  16. Miller CJ, Mıller SR, Trampush J, McKay KE, Newcorn JH, Halperin JM: Family and cognitive factors: modeling risk form aggression in children with ADHD.

    J Am Acad Chıld Adolesc Psychiatry 2006, 45:355-363. Publisher Full Text OpenURL

     

  17. Khaleque A, Rohner RP: Perceived Parental Acceptance-Rejection and Psychological Adjustment: A Meta-Analysis of Cross-Cultural and Intracultural Studies.

    J Marriage Fam 2002, 64:54-64. Publisher Full Text OpenURL

     

  18. Gokler B, Unal F, Pehlivanturk B, CengelKultur E, Akdemir D, Taner Y: Reliability and Validity of Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-age Children-Present and Lifetime Version-Turkish Version (K-SADS-PL-T) [in Turkish].

    Cocuk ve Genclik Ruh Sagligi Derg (Turk J Child Adolesc Ment Health) 2004, 11:109-116. OpenURL

     

  19. Savaşır I, Şahin N: Wechsler Çocuklar İçin Zeka Ölçeği. Ankara: Türk Psikologlar Derneği Yayını; 1994. OpenURL

     

  20. Turgay A: Disruptive Behavior Disorders Child and Adolescent Screening and Rating Scales for Children, Adolescents, Parents and Teachers. West Bloomfield (Michigan): Integrative Therapy Institute Publication; 1994. OpenURL

     

  21. Ercan ES, Amado S, Somer O, Cikoglu S: Development of a test battery for the assessment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder [in Turkish].

    Cocuk ve Genclik Ruh Sagligi Derg (Turk J Child Adolesc Ment Health) 2001, 8:132-144. OpenURL

     

  22. Dupaul GJ, Eckert TL, McGoey KE: Interventions for students with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: one size not fit all.

    Sch Psychol Rev 1997, 26:369-381. OpenURL

     

  23. Magnusson P, Smari J, Greatasdottir H, Prandardottir H: Attention deficit/hyperactivity symptoms in Icelandic school children: assessment with the attention deficit/hyperactivity rating scale.

    Scand J Psychol 1999, 40:301-306. PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text OpenURL

     

  24. Halperin JM, McKay KE, Grayson RH, Newcorn JH: Reliability, validity, and preliminary normative data for the Children’s Aggression Scale-Teacher Version.

    J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatr 2003, 42:965-971. Publisher Full Text OpenURL

     

  25. Halperin JM, McKay KE, Newcorn JH: Development, reliability, and validity of the children’s aggression scale-parent version.

    J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatr 2002, 41:245-252. Publisher Full Text OpenURL

     

  26. Achenbach TM, Edelbrock CS: Manual for the child behavior checklist and revised child behavior profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; 1983. OpenURL

     

  27. Erol N, Arslan BL, Akçakin M: Eunethydis: European Approaches to Hyperkinetic Disorder. Zurich: Fotorotar; 1995. OpenURL

     

  28. Jöreskog KG: New developments in LISREL: Analysis of ordinal variables using polychoric correlations and weighted least squares.

    Qual Quant 1990, 24:387-404. Publisher Full Text OpenURL

     

  29. Muthén BO: A general structural equation model with dichotomous, ordered categorical, and continuous latent variable indicators.

    Psychometrika 1984, 49:115-132. Publisher Full Text OpenURL

     

  30. Jöreskog KG, Sörbom D: LISREL 8: Structural equation modeling with the SIMPLIS command language. Chicago: Scientific Software International; 1993. OpenURL

     

  31. Kline RB: Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. 3rd edition. New York: The Guilford Press; 2011. OpenURL

     

  32. Byrne BM: A primer of LISREL: Basic applications and programming for confirmatory factor analytic models. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1989. OpenURL

     

  33. Carmines EG, McIver JP: Analyzing models with unobserved variables. In Edited by Bohrnstedt GW, Borgatta EF. Current issues. Beverly Hills: Sage; 1981. OpenURL

     

  34. Marsh HW, Hocevar D: Application of confirmatory factor analysis to the study of self-concept: First and higher-order factor models and their invariance across groups.

    Psychol Bull 1985, 97:562-582. OpenURL

     

  35. Bollen KA: Structural equations with latent variables. New York: Wiley; 1989. OpenURL

     

  36. Tucker LR, Lewis C: A reliability coefficient for maximum likelihood factor analysis.

    Psychometrika 1973, 38:1-10. Publisher Full Text OpenURL

     

  37. Hu LT, Bentler PM: Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives.

    Struct Equ Model 1995, 6:1-55. OpenURL

     

  38. Browne MW, Cudeck R: Testing structural equation models. In Alternative ways of assessing model fit. Edited by Bollen KA, Long JS. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1993:136-162.OpenURL

     

  39. Faraone SV, Biederman J, Keenan K, Tsuang MT: A family- genetic study of girls with DSM-III attention deficit disorder.

    Am J Psychiat 1991, 148:112-117. PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text OpenURL

     

  40. Stewart M, deBlois S: Father-son resemblances in aggressive and antisocial behavior.

    Br J Psychiatr 1983, 143:310-311. OpenURL

     

  41. Jaffee SR, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Taylor A: Life with (or without) father: the benefits of living with two biological parents depend on the father’s antisocial behavior.

    Child Dev 2003, 74:109-126. PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text OpenURL

     

  42. Yeh MT, Chen P, Raine A, Bakre LA, Jacobson KC: Child psychopatic traits moderate relationships between parental affect and child aggression.

    J Am Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2011, 50:1054-1064. Publisher Full Text OpenURL

     

  43. Cartwright KL, Bitsakou P, Daley D, Gramzow RH, Psychogiou L, Simonoff E, Thompson MJ, Sonuga-Barke EJ: Disentagling child and family influences on maternal expressed emotion toward children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.

    J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2011, 50:1042-1053. PubMed Abstract |Publisher Full Text OpenURL

     

  44. Lifford KJ, Harold GT, Thapar A: Parent–child hostility and child ADHD symptoms: a genetically sensitive and longitudinal analysis.

    J Child Psychol Psychiatr 2009, 50:1468-1476. Publisher Full Text OpenURL

     

  45. Ercan ES, Somer O, Amado S, Thompson D: Parental recall of preschool behavior related to ADHD and disruptive behavior disorder.

    Child Psychiatry and Hum Dev 2009, 35:299-313. OpenURL


Artigo original:

http://www.capmh.com/content/8/1/15

O portal Psiquiatria Infantil.com.br já recebeu

49.743.302 visitantes